h1

h2

h3

h4

h5
h6
% IMPORTANT: The following is UTF-8 encoded.  This means that in the presence
% of non-ASCII characters, it will not work with BibTeX 0.99 or older.
% Instead, you should use an up-to-date BibTeX implementation like “bibtex8” or
% “biber”.

@PHDTHESIS{Hlden:972949,
      author       = {Hülden, Frank},
      othercontributors = {Intemann, Frauke and Schroeder, Michael},
      title        = {{C}hemie bilingual eine quasiexperimentelle
                      {V}ergleichsstudie zur sachfachlichen, (fremd-)sprachlichen
                      und motivationalen {W}irksamkeit eines bilingualen {M}oduls
                      im {F}ach {C}hemie},
      school       = {Rheinisch-Westfälische Technische Hochschule Aachen},
      type         = {Dissertation},
      address      = {Aachen},
      publisher    = {RWTH Aachen University},
      reportid     = {RWTH-2023-10489},
      pages        = {1 Online-Ressource : Illustrationen},
      year         = {2023},
      note         = {Veröffentlicht auf dem Publikationsserver der RWTH Aachen
                      University; Dissertation, Rheinisch-Westfälische Technische
                      Hochschule Aachen, 2023},
      abstract     = {The present work focuses on the academic, (foreign)
                      language, and motivational effectiveness of a bilingual
                      module in the field of Chemistry. The study is designed as a
                      quasi-experimental comparative study using a pre-, post-,
                      and follow-up test design. It compares the performance of
                      CLIL (Content and Language Integrated Learning) and non-CLIL
                      students before, after, and six weeks following the module.
                      A total of six eighth-grade classes from a North
                      Rhine-Westphalian Gymnasium participated in the main study
                      (N=149). Three classes were taught bilingually (experimental
                      group), and three classes were taught in German (control
                      group). The sample sizes were roughly equal, with N=74 in
                      the experimental group and N=75 in the control group. Two
                      eighth-grade classes from the same school participated in
                      the pilot study (N=52). In total, 201 students were involved
                      in the study. The students had no prior experience in
                      bilingual education and were not selected based on their
                      suitability for bilingual education. Seven hypotheses were
                      formulated for the three objectives of the study, and they
                      were subsequently tested using inferential statistical
                      methods. Data was collected using ten tests and
                      questionnaires. Initially, the question of whether bilingual
                      Chemistry education is too difficult, leading to
                      subject-specific deficits, was examined (Hypothesis 1).
                      Based on the results of the Chemistry tests, no
                      statistically significant performance differences were found
                      between CLIL and non-CLIL students. Bilingually taught
                      students achieved comparable results to regular students,
                      both immediately following the module and six weeks later.
                      Although the subject-specific performance of CLIL students,
                      on average, matched that of regular students, significant
                      differences were observed for individual students. The study
                      revealed statistically significant influences of general
                      chemistry proficiency on performance in the module. While
                      low-performing students showed greater deficits in bilingual
                      education compared to regular education, this was not the
                      case for high-performing students. On the contrary, the
                      study showed that students who excelled in chemistry
                      achieved even better results in bilingual education than in
                      regular education. Additionally, the students' English
                      self-concept had a statistically significant influence on
                      performance in the module. In the course of the study, not
                      only was subject-specific knowledge acquisition (Cognitive
                      Domain I) evaluated but also performance related to higher
                      cognitive operations (Cognitive Domains II and III). It was
                      also investigated whether students participating in
                      bilingual education performed better in cognitively
                      demanding tasks compared to regular students and whether
                      advantages in memorization occurred because content was
                      processed more deeply (Hypothesis 2). However, statistically
                      significant differences in the results of tasks in Cognitive
                      Domains II and III of the Chemistry test were not found. The
                      values for forgetting, measured as the difference between
                      the results of the post-test and follow-up test, were nearly
                      identical in both groups. Therefore, neither the superiority
                      of CLIL students in cognitively demanding tasks nor better
                      memorization performance could be demonstrated.The role of
                      foreign language competence in subject-specific learning was
                      also examined. It was assumed that subject-specific learning
                      does not work below a certain foreign language competence
                      threshold and that better results are expected in regular
                      education. Moreover, it was assumed that there is an upper
                      limit of foreign language competence, above which bilingual
                      education leads to better results (Hypothesis 3). However,
                      no statistically significant influences on subject-specific
                      performance were found for English competencies in this
                      study. Additionally, the question of whether, in the context
                      of dual subject-specific literacy, the German
                      subject-specific vocabulary is as well mastered by CLIL
                      students as by regular students, or whether deficits are
                      expected in CLIL students because the instruction is
                      primarily conducted in a foreign language, was explored
                      (Hypothesis 4). The data analysis yielded no statistically
                      significant differences in either receptive or productive
                      subject-specific vocabulary. Rather, the average gain for
                      CLIL students was only slightly smaller than that of regular
                      students. In the past, bilingual Chemistry education has
                      been criticized for not contributing enough to the
                      development of general language competencies in the foreign
                      language, as instruction primarily involves abstract
                      scientific communication. Therefore, this study also
                      investigated the extent to which general language content is
                      part of the acquired foreign language vocabulary (Hypothesis
                      5). The results showed that an average of 45 new English
                      vocabulary words were learned, two-thirds of which were
                      specialized vocabulary (30 words), and one-third was general
                      vocabulary (15 words). The ratio of specialized vocabulary
                      to general vocabulary was 2:1. Although specialized
                      vocabulary dominated, the increase in English vocabulary was
                      considerable and, overall, greater than in language classes.
                      With the introduction of bilingual education, the hope was
                      to motivate students who are less interested in natural
                      sciences and more interested in languages to engage in the
                      natural sciences. Finally, the motivation of CLIL and
                      regular students was compared. This also addressed whether
                      language learners interested in natural sciences are
                      demotivated by the use of a foreign language (Hypotheses 6
                      and 7). The analysis of the subscales for intrinsic
                      motivation, as per Deci and Ryan (2000), showed that the
                      mean values for interest in the module and perceived
                      usefulness were statistically significantly higher among
                      CLIL students compared to those taught in German. CLIL
                      students rated the CLIL instruction as more interesting and
                      useful. Statistically significant differences were also
                      found in terms of pressure and perceived competence between
                      the CLIL and non-CLIL groups. CLIL students felt greater
                      pressure and considered themselves less competent compared
                      to non-CLIL students. However, the results were the same in
                      all interest groups, and no statistically significant
                      differences were found. Nevertheless, when examining not the
                      general language interest but the specific interest in
                      English, it was found that this was significantly correlated
                      not only with interest in CLIL instruction but also with
                      perceived pressure. Students with a strong interest in
                      English showed greater interest in CLIL instruction and less
                      pressure. On the other hand, it was observed that the
                      Chemistry self-concept negatively correlated with pressure,
                      and students with a strong Chemistry self-concept felt
                      significantly more pressure in CLIL instruction when they
                      believed they were less talented in foreign languages. This
                      may be an indication of contrasting effects according to
                      Marsh's Internal/External Frame of Reference model (1986).
                      Accordingly, students who consider themselves good in
                      natural sciences assume that they are less skilled in
                      foreign languages. In instruction primarily conducted in a
                      foreign language, they consequently feel more pressure.},
      cin          = {793720},
      ddc          = {370},
      cid          = {$I:(DE-82)793720_20140620$},
      typ          = {PUB:(DE-HGF)11},
      doi          = {10.18154/RWTH-2023-10489},
      url          = {https://publications.rwth-aachen.de/record/972949},
}